Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research, ISSN - 0973 - 709X

Users Online : 66181

AbstractMaterial and MethodsResultsDiscussionConclusionAcknowledgementReferencesDOI and Others
Article in PDF How to Cite Citation Manager Readers' Comments (0) Audio Visual Article Statistics Link to PUBMED Print this Article Send to a Friend
Advertisers Access Statistics Resources

Dr Mohan Z Mani

"Thank you very much for having published my article in record time.I would like to compliment you and your entire staff for your promptness, courtesy, and willingness to be customer friendly, which is quite unusual.I was given your reference by a colleague in pathology,and was able to directly phone your editorial office for clarifications.I would particularly like to thank the publication managers and the Assistant Editor who were following up my article. I would also like to thank you for adjusting the money I paid initially into payment for my modified article,and refunding the balance.
I wish all success to your journal and look forward to sending you any suitable similar article in future"



Dr Mohan Z Mani,
Professor & Head,
Department of Dermatolgy,
Believers Church Medical College,
Thiruvalla, Kerala
On Sep 2018




Prof. Somashekhar Nimbalkar

"Over the last few years, we have published our research regularly in Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research. Having published in more than 20 high impact journals over the last five years including several high impact ones and reviewing articles for even more journals across my fields of interest, we value our published work in JCDR for their high standards in publishing scientific articles. The ease of submission, the rapid reviews in under a month, the high quality of their reviewers and keen attention to the final process of proofs and publication, ensure that there are no mistakes in the final article. We have been asked clarifications on several occasions and have been happy to provide them and it exemplifies the commitment to quality of the team at JCDR."



Prof. Somashekhar Nimbalkar
Head, Department of Pediatrics, Pramukhswami Medical College, Karamsad
Chairman, Research Group, Charutar Arogya Mandal, Karamsad
National Joint Coordinator - Advanced IAP NNF NRP Program
Ex-Member, Governing Body, National Neonatology Forum, New Delhi
Ex-President - National Neonatology Forum Gujarat State Chapter
Department of Pediatrics, Pramukhswami Medical College, Karamsad, Anand, Gujarat.
On Sep 2018




Dr. Kalyani R

"Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research is at present a well-known Indian originated scientific journal which started with a humble beginning. I have been associated with this journal since many years. I appreciate the Editor, Dr. Hemant Jain, for his constant effort in bringing up this journal to the present status right from the scratch. The journal is multidisciplinary. It encourages in publishing the scientific articles from postgraduates and also the beginners who start their career. At the same time the journal also caters for the high quality articles from specialty and super-specialty researchers. Hence it provides a platform for the scientist and researchers to publish. The other aspect of it is, the readers get the information regarding the most recent developments in science which can be used for teaching, research, treating patients and to some extent take preventive measures against certain diseases. The journal is contributing immensely to the society at national and international level."



Dr Kalyani R
Professor and Head
Department of Pathology
Sri Devaraj Urs Medical College
Sri Devaraj Urs Academy of Higher Education and Research , Kolar, Karnataka
On Sep 2018




Dr. Saumya Navit

"As a peer-reviewed journal, the Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research provides an opportunity to researchers, scientists and budding professionals to explore the developments in the field of medicine and dentistry and their varied specialities, thus extending our view on biological diversities of living species in relation to medicine.
‘Knowledge is treasure of a wise man.’ The free access of this journal provides an immense scope of learning for the both the old and the young in field of medicine and dentistry as well. The multidisciplinary nature of the journal makes it a better platform to absorb all that is being researched and developed. The publication process is systematic and professional. Online submission, publication and peer reviewing makes it a user-friendly journal.
As an experienced dentist and an academician, I proudly recommend this journal to the dental fraternity as a good quality open access platform for rapid communication of their cutting-edge research progress and discovery.
I wish JCDR a great success and I hope that journal will soar higher with the passing time."



Dr Saumya Navit
Professor and Head
Department of Pediatric Dentistry
Saraswati Dental College
Lucknow
On Sep 2018




Dr. Arunava Biswas

"My sincere attachment with JCDR as an author as well as reviewer is a learning experience . Their systematic approach in publication of article in various categories is really praiseworthy.
Their prompt and timely response to review's query and the manner in which they have set the reviewing process helps in extracting the best possible scientific writings for publication.
It's a honour and pride to be a part of the JCDR team. My very best wishes to JCDR and hope it will sparkle up above the sky as a high indexed journal in near future."



Dr. Arunava Biswas
MD, DM (Clinical Pharmacology)
Assistant Professor
Department of Pharmacology
Calcutta National Medical College & Hospital , Kolkata




Dr. C.S. Ramesh Babu
" Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research (JCDR) is a multi-specialty medical and dental journal publishing high quality research articles in almost all branches of medicine. The quality of printing of figures and tables is excellent and comparable to any International journal. An added advantage is nominal publication charges and monthly issue of the journal and more chances of an article being accepted for publication. Moreover being a multi-specialty journal an article concerning a particular specialty has a wider reach of readers of other related specialties also. As an author and reviewer for several years I find this Journal most suitable and highly recommend this Journal."
Best regards,
C.S. Ramesh Babu,
Associate Professor of Anatomy,
Muzaffarnagar Medical College,
Muzaffarnagar.
On Aug 2018




Dr. Arundhathi. S
"Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research (JCDR) is a reputed peer reviewed journal and is constantly involved in publishing high quality research articles related to medicine. Its been a great pleasure to be associated with this esteemed journal as a reviewer and as an author for a couple of years. The editorial board consists of many dedicated and reputed experts as its members and they are doing an appreciable work in guiding budding researchers. JCDR is doing a commendable job in scientific research by promoting excellent quality research & review articles and case reports & series. The reviewers provide appropriate suggestions that improve the quality of articles. I strongly recommend my fraternity to encourage JCDR by contributing their valuable research work in this widely accepted, user friendly journal. I hope my collaboration with JCDR will continue for a long time".



Dr. Arundhathi. S
MBBS, MD (Pathology),
Sanjay Gandhi institute of trauma and orthopedics,
Bengaluru.
On Aug 2018




Dr. Mamta Gupta,
"It gives me great pleasure to be associated with JCDR, since last 2-3 years. Since then I have authored, co-authored and reviewed about 25 articles in JCDR. I thank JCDR for giving me an opportunity to improve my own skills as an author and a reviewer.
It 's a multispecialty journal, publishing high quality articles. It gives a platform to the authors to publish their research work which can be available for everyone across the globe to read. The best thing about JCDR is that the full articles of all medical specialties are available as pdf/html for reading free of cost or without institutional subscription, which is not there for other journals. For those who have problem in writing manuscript or do statistical work, JCDR comes for their rescue.
The journal has a monthly publication and the articles are published quite fast. In time compared to other journals. The on-line first publication is also a great advantage and facility to review one's own articles before going to print. The response to any query and permission if required, is quite fast; this is quite commendable. I have a very good experience about seeking quick permission for quoting a photograph (Fig.) from a JCDR article for my chapter authored in an E book. I never thought it would be so easy. No hassles.
Reviewing articles is no less a pain staking process and requires in depth perception, knowledge about the topic for review. It requires time and concentration, yet I enjoy doing it. The JCDR website especially for the reviewers is quite user friendly. My suggestions for improving the journal is, more strict review process, so that only high quality articles are published. I find a a good number of articles in Obst. Gynae, hence, a new journal for this specialty titled JCDR-OG can be started. May be a bimonthly or quarterly publication to begin with. Only selected articles should find a place in it.
An yearly reward for the best article authored can also incentivize the authors. Though the process of finding the best article will be not be very easy. I do not know how reviewing process can be improved. If an article is being reviewed by two reviewers, then opinion of one can be communicated to the other or the final opinion of the editor can be communicated to the reviewer if requested for. This will help one’s reviewing skills.
My best wishes to Dr. Hemant Jain and all the editorial staff of JCDR for their untiring efforts to bring out this journal. I strongly recommend medical fraternity to publish their valuable research work in this esteemed journal, JCDR".



Dr. Mamta Gupta
Consultant
(Ex HOD Obs &Gynae, Hindu Rao Hospital and associated NDMC Medical College, Delhi)
Aug 2018




Dr. Rajendra Kumar Ghritlaharey

"I wish to thank Dr. Hemant Jain, Editor-in-Chief Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research (JCDR), for asking me to write up few words.
Writing is the representation of language in a textual medium i e; into the words and sentences on paper. Quality medical manuscript writing in particular, demands not only a high-quality research, but also requires accurate and concise communication of findings and conclusions, with adherence to particular journal guidelines. In medical field whether working in teaching, private, or in corporate institution, everyone wants to excel in his / her own field and get recognised by making manuscripts publication.


Authors are the souls of any journal, and deserve much respect. To publish a journal manuscripts are needed from authors. Authors have a great responsibility for producing facts of their work in terms of number and results truthfully and an individual honesty is expected from authors in this regards. Both ways its true "No authors-No manuscripts-No journals" and "No journals–No manuscripts–No authors". Reviewing a manuscript is also a very responsible and important task of any peer-reviewed journal and to be taken seriously. It needs knowledge on the subject, sincerity, honesty and determination. Although the process of reviewing a manuscript is a time consuming task butit is expected to give one's best remarks within the time frame of the journal.
Salient features of the JCDR: It is a biomedical, multidisciplinary (including all medical and dental specialities), e-journal, with wide scope and extensive author support. At the same time, a free text of manuscript is available in HTML and PDF format. There is fast growing authorship and readership with JCDR as this can be judged by the number of articles published in it i e; in Feb 2007 of its first issue, it contained 5 articles only, and now in its recent volume published in April 2011, it contained 67 manuscripts. This e-journal is fulfilling the commitments and objectives sincerely, (as stated by Editor-in-chief in his preface to first edition) i e; to encourage physicians through the internet, especially from the developing countries who witness a spectrum of disease and acquire a wealth of knowledge to publish their experiences to benefit the medical community in patients care. I also feel that many of us have work of substance, newer ideas, adequate clinical materials but poor in medical writing and hesitation to submit the work and need help. JCDR provides authors help in this regards.
Timely publication of journal: Publication of manuscripts and bringing out the issue in time is one of the positive aspects of JCDR and is possible with strong support team in terms of peer reviewers, proof reading, language check, computer operators, etc. This is one of the great reasons for authors to submit their work with JCDR. Another best part of JCDR is "Online first Publications" facilities available for the authors. This facility not only provides the prompt publications of the manuscripts but at the same time also early availability of the manuscripts for the readers.
Indexation and online availability: Indexation transforms the journal in some sense from its local ownership to the worldwide professional community and to the public.JCDR is indexed with Embase & EMbiology, Google Scholar, Index Copernicus, Chemical Abstracts Service, Journal seek Database, Indian Science Abstracts, to name few of them. Manuscriptspublished in JCDR are available on major search engines ie; google, yahoo, msn.
In the era of fast growing newer technologies, and in computer and internet friendly environment the manuscripts preparation, submission, review, revision, etc and all can be done and checked with a click from all corer of the world, at any time. Of course there is always a scope for improvement in every field and none is perfect. To progress, one needs to identify the areas of one's weakness and to strengthen them.
It is well said that "happy beginning is half done" and it fits perfectly with JCDR. It has grown considerably and I feel it has already grown up from its infancy to adolescence, achieving the status of standard online e-journal form Indian continent since its inception in Feb 2007. This had been made possible due to the efforts and the hard work put in it. The way the JCDR is improving with every new volume, with good quality original manuscripts, makes it a quality journal for readers. I must thank and congratulate Dr Hemant Jain, Editor-in-Chief JCDR and his team for their sincere efforts, dedication, and determination for making JCDR a fast growing journal.
Every one of us: authors, reviewers, editors, and publisher are responsible for enhancing the stature of the journal. I wish for a great success for JCDR."



Thanking you
With sincere regards
Dr. Rajendra Kumar Ghritlaharey, M.S., M. Ch., FAIS
Associate Professor,
Department of Paediatric Surgery, Gandhi Medical College & Associated
Kamla Nehru & Hamidia Hospitals Bhopal, Madhya Pradesh 462 001 (India)
E-mail: drrajendrak1@rediffmail.com
On May 11,2011




Dr. Shankar P.R.

"On looking back through my Gmail archives after being requested by the journal to write a short editorial about my experiences of publishing with the Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research (JCDR), I came across an e-mail from Dr. Hemant Jain, Editor, in March 2007, which introduced the new electronic journal. The main features of the journal which were outlined in the e-mail were extensive author support, cash rewards, the peer review process, and other salient features of the journal.
Over a span of over four years, we (I and my colleagues) have published around 25 articles in the journal. In this editorial, I plan to briefly discuss my experiences of publishing with JCDR and the strengths of the journal and to finally address the areas for improvement.
My experiences of publishing with JCDR: Overall, my experiences of publishing withJCDR have been positive. The best point about the journal is that it responds to queries from the author. This may seem to be simple and not too much to ask for, but unfortunately, many journals in the subcontinent and from many developing countries do not respond or they respond with a long delay to the queries from the authors 1. The reasons could be many, including lack of optimal secretarial and other support. Another problem with many journals is the slowness of the review process. Editorial processing and peer review can take anywhere between a year to two years with some journals. Also, some journals do not keep the contributors informed about the progress of the review process. Due to the long review process, the articles can lose their relevance and topicality. A major benefit with JCDR is the timeliness and promptness of its response. In Dr Jain's e-mail which was sent to me in 2007, before the introduction of the Pre-publishing system, he had stated that he had received my submission and that he would get back to me within seven days and he did!
Most of the manuscripts are published within 3 to 4 months of their submission if they are found to be suitable after the review process. JCDR is published bimonthly and the accepted articles were usually published in the next issue. Recently, due to the increased volume of the submissions, the review process has become slower and it ?? Section can take from 4 to 6 months for the articles to be reviewed. The journal has an extensive author support system and it has recently introduced a paid expedited review process. The journal also mentions the average time for processing the manuscript under different submission systems - regular submission and expedited review.
Strengths of the journal: The journal has an online first facility in which the accepted manuscripts may be published on the website before being included in a regular issue of the journal. This cuts down the time between their acceptance and the publication. The journal is indexed in many databases, though not in PubMed. The editorial board should now take steps to index the journal in PubMed. The journal has a system of notifying readers through e-mail when a new issue is released. Also, the articles are available in both the HTML and the PDF formats. I especially like the new and colorful page format of the journal. Also, the access statistics of the articles are available. The prepublication and the manuscript tracking system are also helpful for the authors.
Areas for improvement: In certain cases, I felt that the peer review process of the manuscripts was not up to international standards and that it should be strengthened. Also, the number of manuscripts in an issue is high and it may be difficult for readers to go through all of them. The journal can consider tightening of the peer review process and increasing the quality standards for the acceptance of the manuscripts. I faced occasional problems with the online manuscript submission (Pre-publishing) system, which have to be addressed.
Overall, the publishing process with JCDR has been smooth, quick and relatively hassle free and I can recommend other authors to consider the journal as an outlet for their work."



Dr. P. Ravi Shankar
KIST Medical College, P.O. Box 14142, Kathmandu, Nepal.
E-mail: ravi.dr.shankar@gmail.com
On April 2011
Anuradha

Dear team JCDR, I would like to thank you for the very professional and polite service provided by everyone at JCDR. While i have been in the field of writing and editing for sometime, this has been my first attempt in publishing a scientific paper.Thank you for hand-holding me through the process.


Dr. Anuradha
E-mail: anuradha2nittur@gmail.com
On Jan 2020

Important Notice

Original article / research
Year : 2023 | Month : May | Volume : 17 | Issue : 5 | Page : CC01 - CC05 Full Version

Effects of Menstrual Cycle on Working Memory and its Correlation with Menstrual Distress Score: A Cross-sectional Study


Published: May 1, 2023 | DOI: https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2023/59757.18009
Kajol Kumari Tulsyan, Soumen Manna, Himani Ahluwalia

1. Postgraduate, Department of Physiology, VMMC and Safdarjung Hospital, New Delhi, India. 2. Associate Professor, Department of Physiology, VMMC and Safdarjung Hospital, New Delhi, India. 3. Professor, Department of Physiology, VMMC and Safdarjung Hospital, New Delhi, India.

Correspondence Address :
Dr. Soumen Manna,
Associate Professor, Department of Physiology, Room No. 323, VMMC and Safdarjung Hospital, New Delhi-110029, India.
E-mail: drsoumen.manna@gmail.com

Abstract

Introduction: The menstrual cycle is a well-known physiological model for studying the effects of ovarian steroid hormones on cognitive functions, such as Working Memory (WM). Hormonal fluctuations during the menstrual cycle also affect menstrual distress related symptoms, which can independently affect cognitive functions. However, data regarding changes in WM functions during different phases of the menstrual cycle and the correlation of various WM functions with menstrual distress-related symptoms have been inconclusive.

Aim: To examine the verbal and visuospatial WM functions during the proliferative (day 10-14) and secretory phase (day 21-25) of the menstrual cycle and to correlate various WM functions with menstrual distress symptom scores.

Materials and Methods: A cross-sectional, observational study was carried out in the Department of Physiology at VMMC and Safdarjung Hospital, New Delhi, India, over a period of 18 months, from November 2020 to May 2022. A total of 40 young adult females with a history of regular menstrual cycles were selected for the study. Computerised software-based dual-task n-back WM tasks were given twice in the same menstrual cycle- first during the proliferative phase (day 10-14) and second during the secretory phase (day 21-25). In addition, a standardised Menstrual Distress Questionnaire (MDQ) based on various menstrual distress symptoms was administered to each subject after completion of the WM task, first during the proliferative phase (day 10-14) and second during the secretory phase (day 21-25). Based on the MDQ, a Menstrual Distress Score (MDS) was calculated. Descriptive statistics, such as mean, median, standard deviation, and mode, were calculated, and Pearson’s coefficient/Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient was used to assess the correlation between WM parameters and MDS. Data were compiled and analysed using the statistical software Graph Pad Prism.

Results: The mean age of the study population was 23.4 years, with an average menstrual cycle length of 30 days. In the WM task, the overall proportion of correct responses across all tasks was significantly better in the secretory phase of the menstrual cycle than in the proliferative phase (p-value=0.040). Similarly, significantly improved performance in WM tasks during the secretory phase was also seen in the overall hit rate of the visual target (p-value=0.020) and auditory targets (p-value=0.044). On the other hand, the correlation of WM parameters with MDS did not show any statistical significance, except for a significant negative correlation (r-value -0.369; p-value=0.019) between the parametric sensitivity (subject’s ability to correctly distinguish a target from a non-target) of auditory WM in the secretory phase and MDS in the proliferative phase.

Conclusion: The visual and auditory WM skills were significantly improved during the secretory phase compared to the proliferative phase of the menstrual cycle in terms of the target hit rate. However, increased MDS had no significant detrimental effect on the performance of WM tasks during the normal menstrual cycle.

Keywords

Follicular phase, Hormonal fluctuation, Luteal phase, Short term memory

The WM is a key cognitive function, that helps an individual’s executive performance (1). The term WM evolved from the concept of “Short-Term Memory (STM),” a cognitive system used for the retention of information over a short delay (2). In contrast to STM, WM implies the capacity of an individual to simultaneously store some information, as well as, manipulate some other information for a short period (3). Classically, two different types of WM have been described, auditory and visuospatial WM and two different systems in the brain process and maintain the information of the auditory and visuospatial WM (4). Visuospatial WM is entrusted with temporary maintenance of an object’s identity and spatial location, whereas, verbal WM temporarily maintains auditory (verbal) information such as words, letters and numbers (3). In addition, various factors affect the WM capacity of an individual, including gender, age and personality (5). Age-related impairment and decline in WM have been reported, and a well established fact (6). It has also been reported that, oestrogen supplements in postmenopausal women improved WM functions (7). This finding highlights a plausible link between WM capacity and the effects of sex steroid hormones (oestrogen and progesterone).

The menstrual cycle is a well known physiological model to study ovarian steroid hormones having an influence on emotion, behaviour, and cognitive functions like WM. Young females in their reproductive age experience dynamic changes in sex steroid hormones (oestrogen and progesterone) during the menstrual cycle. Along with hormonal changes, various premenstrual syndrome symptoms like alteration of mood, behavioural changes, pain and depression are also observed during the menstrual cycle (8).

Earlier studies have demonstrated an association of cognitive functions like WM with hormonal changes during the menstrual cycle (9),(10),(11),(12) and behavioural symptoms in menstrual distress [13-15]. Hormonal fluctuation during the menstrual cycle also modulates the menstrual distressed symptoms apart from cognitive functions like WM discussed above (13),(14),(15). Based on menstrual distress symptoms, the MDQ was developed as a standard method for measuring cyclical menstrual distress and perimenstrual symptoms (16). The MDQ consists of various symptoms experienced during the menstrual cycle. In the questionnaire, 47 different symptoms are categorised into eight subcategories: pain, water retention, autonomic reaction, negative effect, impaired concentration, behavioural change, arousal, and control (16). The MDS was calculated based on this MDQ.

However, the data were inconclusive regarding the change in WM functions during different phases of the menstrual cycle and the correlation of various WM function with menstrual distress symptoms in terms of MDS during the regular menstrual cycles. The inconsistency of the data may be due to several reasons, including the different tasks used for assessing WM functions and diverse subject populations in various studies (17). Also, there was a lack of data in the Indian population regarding the association of WM functions with the different phases of the menstrual cycle. Hence, the present study was planned to compare the visuospatial and verbal WM between the proliferative and the secretory phases of the menstrual cycle as primary objective and to correlate menstrual distress symptoms in the form of MDS with various WM parameters, if any as secondary objective. It had been hypothesised that, verbal and visuospatial WM function would be better in the proliferative phase of the menstrual cycle than in the secretory phase, and menstrual distress would have a detrimental effect on WM functions.

Material and Methods

A cross-sectional, observational study was carried out in the Department of Physiology at VMMC and Safdarjung Hospital, New Delhi, India, over a period of 18 months, from November 2020 to May 2022. Ethical approval was granted by the Institutional Ethics Committee as per approval letter no. (IEC/VMMC/SJH/thesis/2020-11/CC-264).
Sample size calculation: The sample size was calculated using the study of Konishi K et al., as a reference (12). The minimum sample size with 80% power and at 5% level of significance using the formula for comparison of matched pairs by Wilcoxon signed-ranked test was 40 based on the primary task. Simple random sampling method was used for selecting the subjects

Inclusion criteria: 48 right-handed, non obese Body Mass Index (BMI) <30 kg/m2 female resident doctors of the hospital in the age group of 18-28 years, belonging to similar socioeconomic status (subjects self-judged their socioeconomic status) and without any history of significant medical or surgical disease included in the study.

Exclusion criteria: Subjects with irregular menstrual cycle, Polycystic Ovary Syndrome (PCOS), or similar endocrine problems were excluded from the study. Eight subjects, who could not complete the recording twice in both phases of the menstrual cycle were excluded, and the final study population was 40 subjects.

Study Procedure

Two recordings were done from each subject: first on the day 10-14 of the menstrual cycle and second on the day 21-25 of the same menstrual cycle. Confirmation of the menstrual phase by counting the days, the first day of bleeding (menses) was called as day one of the menstrual cycle. After obtaining proper written consent, anthropometric measurements of height by using a stadiometer and weight using a standardised machine were recorded. Then, BMI was calculated using the above parameters. Thereafter, menstrual history in terms of age at menarche, length of the menstrual cycle, duration of bleeding in each cycle, and the approximate amount of blood loss was taken, and they were asked to fill out MDQ MDS was assessed on a four-point scale from 0 (no symptoms) to 3 (severe symptoms) and was expressed in mean and Standard Deviation (SD), based on Moos RH et al., (16).

Computerised software-based verbal and visuospatial WM test using Inquisit 6 software (milliseconds, USA. https://www.millisecond.com/download/library/nback. The task was known as the dual-task n-back task, a go/no go WM performance task with increasing difficulty levels. The implemented procedure in the software was initially described by Jaeggi SM et al., (18). In this task, the participants were presented with two sequences of stimuli in two modalities at the same time:

a) Visual stimulus: a random sequence of blue squares that can be presented in eight different locations on the scren;
b) Auditory stimulus: a random sequence of eight spoken roman letters (Table/Fig 1).

In each trial, one visual (blue square) and one auditory stimulus (random spoken letter) were presented simultaneously. Participants were asked to respond according to the following criteria: for 1-back (N=1) trials, if the location of the square was the same as the one in the previous trial, then it’s a target (blue square), and the subjects had to press “A” on the computer keyboard; if not, then he/she did not have to press “A”. Similarly, if the auditory letter was the same as the previous trial, then it’s a target (random spoken letter), and press “L”; if not, he/she did not have to press “L”. In the same way, for 2-back (N=2) trials, if the location of the square was the same as the one, two trials before, then it’s a target, and the subject had to press “A”; and if the letter was same as the one, two trials before, then it was a target, and the subject had to press “L”. This way 3-back (N=3) trial was also performed. Each dual n-task block consists of 20+ trials.+(plus) trials were the start trial given as a practice session to the subjects. The data of the start trials were not included in the performance of the subjects. Of the actual 20 experimental trials, 4 trials were only of a visual target (where the subject had to press “A” only), 4 trials were only of an auditory target (where the subject had to press “L” only), 2 trials presented both visual, as well as, auditory targets (where subject had to press both “A” and “L” simultaneously), and 10 trials were without any target (where subject didn’t have to press “A” or “L”).

Each trial presents the blue square and random spoken letter for 500 milliseconds, and the next blue square and random spoken letter were presented at an interval of 2500 milliseconds. Participants have the entire 3000 milliseconds to respond by pressing “A” or “L” if they detect a target or both “A” and “L” simultaneously, if they detect two targets. The total time required for the completion of the whole task (all trials) was approximately 25 minutes. Depending on the performance of the subject, the summary data file was collected during the dual-task n-back task. The summary data file contained various parameters of visuospatial and auditory WM such as ‘overall proportion correct’ responses across all trials, “hit rate overall” (‘hit’ means when the subject press ‘A’ for a visual target or ‘L’ for an auditory target) for visual and auditory targets across all trials, ‘false alarm rate overall’ (‘false alarm’ means when subject press ‘A’ or ‘L’ for a non target). ‘Z-scores’ of “hit rate” and “false alarm rate” were also calculated. ‘Z-score’ was an adjustment score calculated as per recommendation by Gregg AP and Sedikides C (19). The adjustment was that, if the ‘hit rate’ or ‘false alarm rata’ was 0 then 0.005 was added to the value of ‘hit rate’ or ‘false alarm rate’, and if the ‘hit rate’ or ‘false alarm rata’ was 1 then 0.995 was added to the value of ‘hit rate’ or ‘false alarm rate’. ‘Parametric sensitivity (d`)’ of both visual and auditory WM was also calculated. A higher value of ‘d’ indicates better performance by correctly distinguishing a target from a non target. A value of d`=0 suggests that it’s a chance performance, whereas, a negative value of d` indicates that, non target was treated as target and target was treated as non target.

Statistical Analysis

Data was compiled and analysed using the statistical software Graph PadPrism. Data were checked for normal distribution using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Statistical significance of quantitative variables between the two groups was carried out by paired Student’s t-test for Gaussian distributed parameters and Wilcoxon signed-rank test for non parametric parameters. Pearson’s coefficient/ Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient was used to assess the correlation between WM parameters and MDS. The significance level was considered as p-value <0.05, Confidence interval as 95% and β=0.2 and power was considered as 80%. Descriptive statistics such as mean, median, standard deviation and mode were calculated and represented with appropriate pictorial diagrams.

Results

The mean age of the study population was 23.4 years and average menstrual cycle length was 30 days (Table/Fig 2). The mean±SD of the ‘overall proportion of correct’ response across all trials was 0.65±0.09 during the proliferative phase and 0.68±0.12 during the secretory phase of the menstrual cycle. The mean±SD of MDS were 11.93±9.49 during the proliferative phase and 17.45±16.98 during the secretory phase of the menstrual cycle. Analysing the data of various WM parameters showed that, the overall proportion of correct responses (p-value=0.040) were significantly different between the proliferative and secretory phases of the menstrual cycle, with improved performance during the secretory phase (Table/Fig 3). Similar, significantly improved performance in WM tasks during the secretory phase of the menstrual cycle was also seen in the overall ‘hit rate’ of visual targets (p-value=0.020) and overall ‘hit rate’ of auditory targets (p-value=0.044) (Table/Fig 4).

Calculated Z-score of the overall visual hit rate (p-value=0.044) and overall auditory hit rate (p-value=0.020) showed improved performance during the secretory phase of the menstrual cycle as compared to the proliferative phase (Table/Fig 4). However, the false alarm rate, and Z-score of the overall false alarm rate of both visual, as well as, auditory WM tasks did not differ between the proliferative and secretory phases of the menstrual cycle. Parametric sensitivity of overall visual responses was significantly better in the secretory phase (p-value=0.042) as compared to the proliferative phase, but ‘parametric sensitivity’ of the overall auditory responses did differ between the two phases of the menstrual cycle. The correlation of WM parameters with MDS showed a statistically significant (p=0.019) negative correlation (r-value=-0.369) between the parametric sensitivity of auditory WM of the secretory phase and MDS of the proliferative phase (Table/Fig 5). All other parameters of WM were insignificantly correlated to the MDS of the menstrual cycle’s secretory and proliferative phases.

Discussion

The WM is a cognitive system for temporarily storing and manipulating remembered information. Intact WM is essential for many higher order cognitive tasks, like reasoning, explanation, and mathematical computation (2). The influence of sex hormones such as oestrogens, progesterone, and testosterone on cognitive functions was established as a reason for sex differences in cognitive tasks, including WM (20),(21). The menstrual cycle is a natural model for understanding these hormonal influences on WM because hormonal flux, particularly oestrogen and progesterone, occur naturally along the menstrual cycle (17),(22). However, the data were inconclusive regarding the change in WM functions during proliferative and secretory phases of the menstrual cycle (12). Moreover, the correlation of various WM function with menstrual distress symptoms in terms of MDS during the regular menstrual cycles were also lacking, particularly in the Indian population. So, in the present study, assessment of the WM functions in two distinct phases of the menstrual cycle (follicular or proliferative phase and secretory, or luteal phase) was done from 40 normally menstruating (average length 30.0±3 days) healthy young adult women in age group of 20-24 years, with a BMI of 22.9±3.8 kg/m2. All women included in the study were right-handed to avoid hemispheric effect of brain on cognition.

In the present study, the WM task parameter ‘overall proportion of correct’ response was significantly better in the secretory phase (day 21-25) of the menstrual cycle than the response in the proliferative phase (day 10-14). Furthermore, audio-visual WM tasks in the form of visual ‘hit rate’ and auditory ‘hit rate’ were also better in the secretory phase in comparison to the proliferative phase, and the difference was statistically significant. These findings indicated improved visual and auditory WM skills in terms of the target ‘hit rate’ during the secretory phase as compared to the proliferative phases of the menstrual cycle. However, the ‘overall false alarm rate’ of both visual and auditory WM was not different between the proliferative and secretory phases. This effect was probably due to the higher progesterone level in the secretory phase of the menstrual cycle because in a normal menstrual cycle, the oestrogen level is low, and the progesterone level is high during the secretory phase (23). The present study’s findings were similar to a study conducted by Hidalgo-Lopez E and Pletzer B. In their study, women performed WM tasks significantly faster during the luteal phase than in the pre-ovulatory phase (24). Similarly, a study by Simic N and Santini M also demonstrated that, the best performance of verbal WM and verbal fluency were seen during the menstrual and mid-luteal phases in comparison to the proliferative phase of the menstrual cycle (25). Another study by Maki PM et al., also showed that, the performance task of implicit memory was better at the mid-luteal than the follicular phase, but performance on a test of explicit memory did not vary across the menstrual cycle (26). The present study also concluded that, improved performance of WM tasks during the proliferative phase was attributed to progesterone but not estradiol (26). It has been shown that, estradiol produces greater activation in the left prefrontal cortex, a region associated with verbal processing and encoding, while progesterone was associated with changes in regional brain activation patterns during a visual memory task (left prefrontal cortex and right hippocampus) (27).

However, Leeners B et al., described a negative association between progesterone level and change in WM from the pre-ovulatory to mid-luteal phase of the menstrual cycle. Although, when the same subjects were followed-up for the consecutive second menstrual, a negative association didn’t replicate (10). Hampson E and Morley EE also demonstrated that, higher oestrogen level rather than progesterone level was associated with better WM performance (28). Along with WM functions, MDS was also assessed for each subject. The central hypothesis of the present study was that, the MDS during different phases of the menstrual cycle had a detrimental effect on verbal and visuospatial WM in young adult females. However, the data didn’t support the above hypothesis. Statistically significant correlations between WM parameters and MDS in both the secretory and proliferative phases of the menstrual cycle were not found in the study. However, a negative correlation of statistically significant between the ‘parametric sensitivity’ of the auditory WM parameter of the secretory phase and MDS of the proliferative phase (p=0.019, r=-0.369). The result was similar to a study by Sundstrom Poromaa I and Gingnell M; and Hartley LR et al., (17),(29). These findings indicated that, the higher MDS in terms of menstrual distress had no detrimental effect on WM performance in young adult women. However, auditory WM performance of secretory phase negatively correlates with MDS of proliferative phase, indicating menstrual distress of proliferative phase may affect auditory WM performance during the secretory phase of menstrual cycle.

Limitation(s)

In the present study, data of only one menstrual cycle at two time points had been collected and hormonal assays was not done. It would be better if follow-up of the same subjects could be done for consecutive 3 to 4 menstrual cycles and data collection could be done at multiple time points within the same menstrual cycle, rather than only in the proliferative and the secretory phases of the menstrual cycle along with hormonal measurement, preferably on a large population. Additionally, in the present study, only subjects with normal menstrual cycle have been used, however, the results from the present study may be compared with the subjects having abnormal mensuration cycle to yield better comparison.

Conclusion

The present study found that, young female subject’s visual and auditory WM skills improved during the secretory phase as compared to proliferative phases of the normal menstrual cycle in terms of ‘target hit rate’. However, the overall ‘false alarm rate’ of both, visual and auditory WM was not different between the proliferative and secretory phases. In addition, increased MDS had no significant detrimental effect on WM tasks in young adult women.

Acknowledgement

The authors would like to thank VMMC and SJH for providing adequate support for the study. Also, to all the volunteeres, who helped in the present study.

References

1.
Engle RW. Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002;11(1):19-23. [crossref]
2.
Aben B, Stapert S, Blokland A. About the distinction between working memory and short-term memory. Front Psychol. 2012;3:301. [crossref][PubMed]
3.
Baddeley A. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:01-29. [crossref][PubMed]
4.
Shipstead Z, Martin JD, Nespodzany A. Visuospatial working memory, auditory discrimination, and attention. Memory. 2019;27(4):568-74. [crossref][PubMed]
5.
Blasiman RN, Was CA. Why Is Working memory performance unstable? A review of 21 factors. Eur J Psychol. 2018;14(1):188-231. [crossref][PubMed]
6.
Daselaar S, Cabeza R. 456 age-related decline in working memory and episodic memory: contributions of the prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobes. In: Ochsner KN, Kosslyn S, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience, Volume 1: Core Topics: Oxford University Press; 2013. Pp. 0. [crossref]
7.
Hampson E. Estrogens, aging, and working memory. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2018;20(12):109. [crossref][PubMed]
8.
O’Brien PM, Backstrom T, Brown C, Dennerstein L, Endicott J, Epperson CN, et al. Towards a consensus on diagnostic criteria, measurement and trial design of the premenstrual disorders: The ISPMD Montreal consensus. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2011;14(1):13-21. [crossref][PubMed]
9.
Kozaki T, Yasukouchi A. Sex differences on components of mental rotation at different menstrual phases. Int J Neurosci. 2009;119(1):59-67. [crossref][PubMed]
10.
Leeners B, Kruger THC, Geraedts K, Tronci E, Mancini T, Ille F, et al. Lack of associations between female hormone levels and visuospatial working memory, divided attention and cognitive bias across two consecutive menstrual cycles. Front Behav Neurosci. 2017;11:120. [crossref][PubMed]
11.
Rosenberg L, Park S. Verbal and spatial functions across the menstrual cycle in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27(7):835-41.[crossref][PubMed]
12.
Konishi K, Kumashiro M, Izumi H, Higuchi Y. Effects of the menstrual cycle on working memory: Comparison of postmenstrual and premenstrual phases. Ind Health. 2008;46(3):253-60. [crossref][PubMed]
13.
Diener D, Greenstein FL, Turnbough PD. Cyclical variation in digit-span and visual-search performance in women differing in the severity of their premenstrual symptoms. Percept Mot Skills. 1992;74(1):67-76. [crossref][PubMed]
14.
Morgan M, Rapkin AJ, D’Elia L, Reading A, Goldman L. Cognitive functioning in premenstrual syndrome. Obstet Gynecol. 1996;88(6):961-66. [crossref][PubMed]
15.
Phillips SM, Sherwin BB. Variations in memory function and sex steroid hormones across the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1992;17(5):497-506. [crossref][PubMed]
16.
Moos RH. The development of a menstrual distress questionnaire. Psychosom Med. 1968;30(6):853-67. [crossref][PubMed]
17.
Sundstrom Poromaa I, Gingnell M. Menstrual cycle influence on cognitive function and emotion processing-from a reproductive perspective. Front Neurosci. 2014;8:380. [crossref][PubMed]
18.
Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(19):6829-33. [crossref][PubMed]
19.
Gregg AP, Sedikides C. Narcissistic fragility: Rethinking its links to explicit and implicit self-esteem. Self and Identity. 2010;9(2):142-61. [crossref]
20.
Hirnstein M, Hugdahl K, Hausmann M. Cognitive sex differences and hemispheric asymmetry: A critical review of 40 years of research. Laterality. 2019;24(2):204-52. [crossref][PubMed]
21.
Kheloui S, Brouillard A, Rossi M, Marin MF, Mendrek A, Paquette D, et al. Exploring the sex and gender correlates of cognitive sex differences. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2021;2021:103452. [crossref][PubMed]
22.
Yamazaki M, Tamura K. The menstrual cycle affects recognition of emotional expressions: An event-related potential study. F1000Res. 2017;6:853. [crossref][PubMed]
23.
Masuda S, Ichihara K, Yamanishi H, Hirano Y, Tanaka Y, Kamisako T, et al. Evaluation of menstrual cycle-related changes in 85 clinical laboratory analytes. Ann Clin Biochem. 2016;53(Pt 3):365-76. [crossref][PubMed]
24.
Hidalgo-Lopez E, Pletzer B. Interactive effects of dopamine baseline levels and cycle phase on executive functions: The role of progesterone. Front Neurosci. 2017;11:403. [crossref][PubMed]
25.
Simic N, Santini M. Verbal and spatial functions during different phases of the menstrual cycle. Psychiatr Danub. 2012;24(1):73-79.
26.
Maki PM, Rich JB, Rosenbaum RS. Implicit memory varies across the menstrual cycle: Estrogen effects in young women. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(5):518-29. [crossref][PubMed]
27.
Berent-Spillson A, Briceno E, Pinsky A, Simmen A, Persad CC, Zubieta JK, et al. Distinct cognitive effects of estrogen and progesterone in menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;59:25-36. [crossref][PubMed]
28.
Hampson E, Morley EE. Estradiol concentrations and working memory performance in women of reproductive age. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(12):2897-904. [crossref][PubMed]
29.
Hartley LR, Lyons D, Dunne M. Memory and menstrual cycle. Ergonomics. 1987;30(1):111-20.[crossref][PubMed]

DOI and Others

DOI: 10.7860/JCDR/2023/59757.18009

Date of Submission: Aug 20, 2022
Date of Peer Review: Oct 25, 2022
Date of Acceptance: Jan 27, 2023
Date of Publishing: May 01, 2023

AUTHOR DECLARATION:
• Financial or Other Competing Interests: None
• Was Ethics Committee Approval obtained for this study? Yes
• Was informed consent obtained from the subjects involved in the study? Yes
• For any images presented appropriate consent has been obtained from the subjects. NA

PLAGIARISM CHECKING METHODS:
• Plagiarism X-checker: Aug 30, 2022
• Manual Googling: Oct 10, 2022
• iThenticate Software: Jan 25, 2023 (7%)

ETYMOLOGY: Author Origin

JCDR is now Monthly and more widely Indexed .
  • Emerging Sources Citation Index (Web of Science, thomsonreuters)
  • Index Copernicus ICV 2017: 134.54
  • Academic Search Complete Database
  • Directory of Open Access Journals (DOAJ)
  • Embase
  • EBSCOhost
  • Google Scholar
  • HINARI Access to Research in Health Programme
  • Indian Science Abstracts (ISA)
  • Journal seek Database
  • Google
  • Popline (reproductive health literature)
  • www.omnimedicalsearch.com