JCDR - Register at Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research
Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research, ISSN - 0973 - 709X
Paediatrics Section DOI : 10.7860/JCDR/2016/16647.7283
Year : 2016 | Month : Feb | Volume : 10 | Issue : 02 Full Version Page : SC14 - SC17

Changes in Habitual and Active Sagittal Posture in Children and Adolescents with and without Visual Input – Implications for Diagnostic Analysis of Posture

Oliver Ludwig1, Carola Mazet2, Dirk Mazet3, Annette Hammes4, Eduard Schmitt5

1 Scientific Head, AG Kid-Check, Institute of Sport Sciences, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany.
2 Senior Physiotherapist, AG Kid-Check, Institute of Sport Sciences, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany.
3 Senior Researcher, AG Kid-Check, Institute of Sport Sciences, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany.
4 Senior Researcher, AG Kid-Check, Endokrinologikum, Saarbrücken, Germany.
5 Medical Head AG Kid-Check, Klinik für Orthopädie und orthop. Chirurgie, Universitätsklinikum Homburg, Germany.


NAME, ADDRESS, E-MAIL ID OF THE CORRESPONDING AUTHOR: Dr. Oliver Ludwig, Saarland University, Institute of Sport Sciences, Campus Geb. B 8.1, 66041 Saarbruecken, Germany. E-mail : Oliver.ludwig1@uni-saarland.de
Abstract

Introduction

Poor posture in children and adolescents has a prevalence of 22-65% and is suggested to be responsible for back pain. To assess posture, photometric imaging of sagittal posture is widely used, but usually only habitual posture positions (resting position with minimal muscle activity) are analysed.

Aim

The objective of this study was 1) to investigate possible changes in posture-describing parameters in the sagittal plane, when the subjects changed from a habitual passive posture to an actively corrected posture, and 2) to investigate the changes in posture parameters when an actively corrected posture was to be maintained with closed eyes.

Materials and Methods

In a group of 216 male children and adolescents (average 12.4 ± 2.5 years, range 7.0 – 17.6 years), six sagittal posture parameters (body tilt BT, trunk incline TI, posture index PI, horizontal distances between ear, shoulder and hip and the perpendicular to the ankle joint) were determined by means of photometric imaging in an habitual passive posture position, in an actively erect posture with eyes open, and in active stance with eyes closed. The change in these parameters during the transition between the posture positions was analysed statistically (dependent t-Test or Wilcoxon-Test) after Bonferroni correction (p<0.004).

Results

When moving from a habitual passive to an active posture BT, TI, PI, dEar, dShoulder, and dHip decreased significantly(p< 0.004). When the eyes were closed, only the perpendicular distances (dEar, dShoulder, and dHip) increased significantly. The parameters that describe the alignment of the trunk divtions in relation to each other (BT, TI, PI), remained unchanged in both actively regulated posture positions.

Conclusion

Changes in sagittal posture parameters that occur when a habitual passive posture switches into an active posture or when an active posture is to be maintained while the eyes are closed can be used for diagnostic purposes regarding poor posture and posture regulation.

Keywords

Introduction

Poor posture is a common problem in children and adolescents and has a prevalence of 22-65% [13]. The quantification of body posture in the sagittal plane, which is important for clinical diagnosis, can be measured by means of angle and distance parameters [47]. In posture assessment in clinical daily practice, it is important to define which specific posture is being assessed by the researcher.

Both proprio-receptive and also visual and vestibular information are involved in posture control [8]. Thus, the quality of posture being determined clinically is dependent on the influence of receptors, of the central nervous processing system, and of the muscles. Differences between passive and active postures and between postures with open or closed eyes could be relevant for the assessment of poor posture and at the same time lead to new starting points for therapeutic interventions.

Aim

The first objective of the present trial was to investigate whether the geometric parameters describing posture in children and adolescents in the sagittal plane change when the subjects try to correct their posture by activating muscles.

The study also investigates whether changes in the posture parameters occur when an actively corrected posture is to be maintained and the eyes are closed.

Materials and Methods

Two hundred sixteen male children and adolescents took part in the study (12.4 ± 2.5 years, 47.5 ± 15.8 kg, 158.3± 17.5 cm). The subjects were recruited by means of newspaper ads within the framework of an interdisciplinary university research project that explores weak posture in children (www.kidcheck.de). Parents who thought that their children suffered from weak posture and who wanted their children to be examined attended an examination appointment at our posture analysis laboratory (Saarland University, Germany). In the years 2012 – 2014 a total of 348 children and adolescents between 7.0 and 17.6 years participated in the project. For this study, posture data of all 216 male participants was analysed post-hoc.

The participants and their parents or guardians were informed prior to the trial, in accordance with the requirements of the Declaration of Helsinki, on the trial objective and trial procedure and gave their written informed consent.

All the posture parameters were calculated based on posture images of the sagittal plane. This is a practical, reliable and valid examination method [9,10]. For this, the test subjects had to undress and high-contrast marker points or marker spheres (12 mm in diameter) were stuck onto the anatomical reference points indicated in [Table/Fig-1]. The subjects were placed sideways in front of a measuring wall looking straight ahead. A total of three posture images were taken using a high-resolution camera (Olympus SP510UZ, resolution 2304 x 3072 pixels), which was placed on a tripod at hip-height in each case. The measuring wall was used to calibrate the camera image on a horizontal plane.

Anatomical landmarks that are used in the study: 1 – skull, 2 – auditory canal, 3 – acromion, 4 – distal sternum, 5 – maximum of thoracic kyphosis, 6 – SIAS, 7 – maximum of lumbar lordosis, 8 – trochanter major, 9 – malleolus lateralis, 10 – sole of foot

The subjects were asked to take the following postures one after another [Table/Fig-2]:

Posture positions that were analysed in the study (boy, 15 y): a. passive habitual posture, subject standing relaxed; hip, shoulder and ear are in front of the plumb line (yellow); b. actively erected posture, eyes open; note that all horizontal marker distances are reduced; c. actively erected posture, eyes closed; hip, shoulder and ear distances increase again (for description of posture positions, see text).

Relaxed habitual posture (passive posture PA): The subjects were told to stand in a relaxed way, facing forwards with their arms hanging down at the sides of their bodies.

Actively erect posture with eyes open (Aeo): The subjects were told to try to erect their posture by actively tensing their muscles. To give them a model they were told: “Imagine that you are a puppet and are being pulled up from your head by means of a string.” The subjects were told to continue to breathe normally.

Actively erect posture with eyes closed (Aec): The subjects were told to maintain the actively erect posture, continue to breathe normally and to close their eyes. After 60 seconds the third posture image was taken.

The image data was transferred to a computer. With the aid of the posture analysis programme Corpus concepts (AFG, Idar-Oberstein, Germany), the following posture parameters were determined [Table/Fig-3]:

Posture parameters in the sagittal plane: A) ear plumb line distance (dE), shoulder plumb line distance (dS), hip plumb line distance (dH); b) trunk incline (TI), upper body tilt (BT); c) plumb line distances that are used to calculate the posture index (see text).

- Horizontal distances between the marker points of the hip, shoulder and ear and the perpendicular to the ankle joint (perpendicular distance).

- Upper body tilt (angle of the junction of marker 5 and 7 from the vertical).

- Trunk incline (angle of the junction of marker 5 and 8 from the vertical).

- Posture index.

The posture index according to Fröhner [11] is a parameter which gives a summarised assessment of trunk alignment, by calculating the distances of four body points to the perpendicular through the ankle joint. The caudal tip of the sternum, the point of maximum lumbar lordosis, the point of maximum thoracic kyphosis and the Spina iliaca anterior superior (SIAS) act as reference points. The posture index is calculated using (dK+dA) / (dB+dL), where dK = horizontal distance between thoracal kyphosis and plumb line, dA = horizontal distance between SIAS and plumb line, dB = horizontal distance between breastbone and plumb line, dL = horizontal distance between lumbar lordosis and plumb line. A stable posture is indicated by values between 1.0 and 1.3 [11].

Thehorizontal perpendicular distances were scaled according to the subject’s body height (vertical distance between marker 1 and 10 in [Table/Fig-1].

Statistical Analysis

The statistical parameters were calculated using the software WinSTAT for Windows Excel. Differences between passive habitual posture (PA), active with open eyes (Aeo) and active with closed eyes (Aec) were tested for significance with the t-Test or Wilcoxon-Test after testing for normal distribution of differences. The p-value was set at 0.05. After Bonferroni correction of the 12 difference hypotheses the significance level was 0.004.

Results

When changing from passive habitual (PA) to active posture (Aeo) all examined parameters changed significantly [Table/Fig-4]. When standing in an activated posture all perpendicular distances decreased, as did the trunk incline of the torso and upper body [Table/Fig-5]. This indicates that the subjects reduced the forward flexion of their body. The biggest changes of all perpendicular distances were observed for the ear marker (-4.44 ± 2.43 % height for PA versus -3.04 ± 2.60 % height for Aeo). As body tilt (BT) decreased from 13.91° ± 4.88° to 12.85° ± 5.0°, the lumbar lordosis was reduced when posture was activated. Beyond that, the significantly reduced posture index (PI) indicates a straighter position of the upper body in active posture.

Posture parameters in the three test situations (mean ±standard deviation), N=216.

Posturepositionpassiveactiveeyes openactiveeyescloseddifferences
PAAeoAec
mean stdevmean stdevmean stdevPA - AeoAeo - AecPA - Aec
dHip(% height)-2.8±1.82-1.94±1.79-2.38±1.75* w* w* w
dShoulder(% height)-3.18±2.39-2.27±2.46-3.23±2.74* w* w0.699 w
dEar[% height]-4.44±2.43-3.04±2.60-4.00±1.74* w* w0.008 w
TI Trunk incline (°)16.8±4.1015.01±3.8315.05±4.15* t0.468 t* t
BT Body tilt(°)13.91±4.8812.85±5.0012.80±5.66* t0.922 t* t
PI Posture index (-)1.12±0.201.02±0.211.03±0.22* t0.059 t* t

Negative values for perpendicular distances mean that the marker point is located in front of the plumb line. The three columns to the right indicate significant differences (p-values) between the three posture positions (PA – passive posture, Aeo – active posture with open eyes, Aec – active posture with closed eyes): * p<0.004; t: t-Test, w: Wilcoxon-Test. dHip, dShoulder, dEar are the horizontal distances between the marker points and the plumb line through the ankle marker (other parameters see text).


Box plots of the calculated posture parameters for the three test situations. The boxes span the interquartile range (25 and 75 percentile); whiskers mark the 5 and 95 percentile. The horizontal bar marks the median, and black squares mark outliers. Red dotted lines link the medians.

Upon closing the eyes, all the perpendicular distances (dHip, dShoulder, dEar) again increased significantly [Table/Fig-4], column Aeo-Aec). While the forward flexion of the body was reduced when posture was voluntary activated, this correction disappeared after the eyes were closed [Table/Fig-5 d-f]. The perpendicular shoulder distance (dShoulder, -3.23 ± 2.74 % height) and ear distance (dEar, -4.00 ± 1.74% height) almost reached the base level of passive habitual posture [Table/Fig-5 d+e].

The parameters describing the alignment of the trunk segments in relation to one another (posture index PI, body tilt BT, trunk incline TI) remained the same in the two actively controlled posture positions [Table/Fig-5 a-c].

Discussion

Poor posture in children and adolescents is suggested to be responsible for back pain both in young and adult age [4]. Therefore, proper posture assessment is crucial to initiate appropriate clinical intervention [9]. However, the question arises which posture positions should ideally be measured. In our study, both passive and active posture positions of children and adolescents were examined and compared. We were able to show that significant differences exist between active and passive posture positions, and that changes in posture occur when the eyes were closed. Therefore, these three posture positions should be compared in clinical posture assessment.

Posture control as a regulatory process: In order to understand the importance of distinguishing these posture positions, it is helpful to realise how the central nervous system (CNS) regulates posture and what measurable changes in trunk alignment occur during that process.

Posture is a result of a complex regulatory process, in which active structures (muscles) and passive structures (ligaments, tendons, joints) interact [12, 13]. In the relaxed passive posture position most of our subjects showed a slight tendency to tilt forwards in relation to the plumb line to the ankle, which is reflected geometrically in the increase in the perpendicular distances from caudal to cranial. This is consistent with other studies which show that the regulation of the centre of mass is primarily carried out through the ankle joint [14,15].

When moving from an habitual passive to an active posture, it is well known that a measurable displacement of the trunk segments in relation to each other takes place [16], caused by increased muscle activity [17]. This process was assessed in our study by comparing the changes in the sagittal posture parameters.

Posture control without the visual system: The changes that occurred when the eyes were closed are interesting. An increased forward flexion of the body could be observed. The alignment of the trunk segments to one another remained unchanged, even with the eyes closed, as shown by the constant posture index, trunk incline and upper body tilt. This initially seems to be contradictory but can be explained by the posture control mechanisms in the CNS. The main receptor systems involved in posture control are the proprio-receptive information systems (muscle, tendon and joint receptors [18], mechano-receptive information systems (pressure sensors in the skin) and vestibular and visual information systems [19]. During the process of sensory integration the CNS uses this information from receptors to perceive the current body posture and to generate muscle reactions if a posture deviation is noted [20]. When the eyes are closed, the visual receptor system (the ‘optical analyser’) is turned off from a neurophysiological point of view. With the loss of visual information the CNS has to rely on the remaining receptor information systems to guarantee the upright stance of the body and the stabilisation of the trunk segments in relation to one another [21].

A forward displacement of the body, such as that observed when the subject’s eyes were closed, at the same time generates a greater mechanical pressure on the forefoot. This may be interpreted as a strategy to provoke a greater level of receptor information from the mechano-receptors, in order to allow a more effective perception of the position of the body in the sagittal plane, despite the eyes being closed.

These results are consistent with the research done by Collins et al., who hypothesized that switching off the visual sense increases the level of muscular activity across the joints of the lower limb, and thus increases the stiffness of the musculoskeletal system [22]. This is medically relevant, since Dolphens et al., were able to show that the forward displacement of the body is an important parameter that is linked to the appearance of complaints [5]. The quality of control of the forward displacement of the body is largely dependent on vision. Peterka could show that during quiet stance with eyes open normal subjects derive about one-third of their orientation information from vision [8].

Relevance for clinical practice: In terms of medical posture assessment it can be deduced that the observed change of the active posture once the eyes are closed allows us to draw significant conclusions regarding the role of the optical analyser in the area of posture control [23]. The observed worsening in posture which occurs when the eyes are closed could thus be interpreted to quantify the deficit of the proprio-receptive awareness of the body position on the sagittal plane. Therefore, a comparison between the active posture with open eyes and closed eyes can provide useful information for the physical therapist to decide which kind of exercise a child should perform to improve its poor posture, and its proprio-receptive awareness of the body position. If the posture worsens when the eyes are closed, an exercise program focusing on body awareness could be helpful.

By comparing the posture parameters in the three posture positions examined in this study, physicians are able to make differential diagnostic statements 1. About the ability of intentionally activating the muscles relevant to posture, 2. About the precision of posture control, and 3. About the significance of the visual system for posture control. Therapeutic intervention aimed at treating poor posture can be established using these results.

Our results may be summarized as follows

The significant change in all the parameters recorded in the sagittal plane indicates that these are suitable for assessing body posture and for measuring differences between passive and active posture of children and adolescents.

For medical diagnosis this also implies that there is a requirement to always examine both passive and active posture positions and to compare the two.

Posture transition when the eyes are closed provides important information about the performance of proprio-receptive information processing (body awareness) of the subject examined.

Conclusion

In clinical assessment of posture of children and adolescents, habitual passive posture and active posture with open and closed eyes are recommended to be compared by measuring sagittal posture parameters. The comparison of these parameters enables conclu-sions about the quality of posture control and the proprio-receptive awareness of posture.

Negative values for perpendicular distances mean that the marker point is located in front of the plumb line. The three columns to the right indicate significant differences (p-values) between the three posture positions (PA – passive posture, Aeo – active posture with open eyes, Aec – active posture with closed eyes): * p<0.004; t: t-Test, w: Wilcoxon-Test. dHip, dShoulder, dEar are the horizontal distances between the marker points and the plumb line through the ankle marker (other parameters see text).

References

[1]Gh Maghsoud E, Alilou A, Ghafurinia S, Fereydounnia S, Prevalence of faulty posture in children and youth from a rural region in Iran Biomedical Human Kinetics 2012 :121  [Google Scholar]

[2]Bogdanović Z, Marković Ž, Presence of lordotic poor posture resulted by absence of sport in primary school children Acta Kinesiologica 2010 4(1):63-66.  [Google Scholar]

[3]Kopecký M, Posture Assessment in Children of the School Age Group (7-15 Years of Age) in the Olomouc Region Acta Universitatis Palackianae Olomucensis Gymnica 2004 34(2):19-29.  [Google Scholar]

[4]Smith A, O’Sullivan P, Straker L, Classification of Sagittal Thoraco-Lumbo-Pelvic Alignment of the Adolescent Spine in Standing and Its Relationship to Low Back Pain Spine 2008 33(19):2101-07.  [Google Scholar]

[5]Dolphens M, Cagnie B, Coorevits P, Vleeming A, Danneels L, Classification system of the normal variation in sagittal standing plane alignement Spine 2013 38(16):E1003-12.  [Google Scholar]

[6]Mac-Thiong J-M, Labelle H, Roussouly P, Pediatric sagittal alignment Eur Spine J 2011 20(Suppl 5):586-90.  [Google Scholar]

[7]Roussouly P, Gollogly S, Berthonnaud E, Dimnet J, Classification of the normal variation in the sagittal alignment of the human lumbar spine and pelvis in the standing position Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 2005 30(3):346-53.  [Google Scholar]

[8]Peterka R, Sensorimotor integration in human postural control Journal of Neurophysiology 2002 88(3):1097-118.  [Google Scholar]

[9]do Rosario JL, Photographic analysis of human posture: a literature review J Bodyw Mov Ther 2014 18(1):56-61.  [Google Scholar]

[10]Singla D, Veqar Z, Methods of Postural Assessment Used for Sports Persons Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research: JCDR 2014 8(4):LE01-LE4.  [Google Scholar]

[11]Fröhner G, [Objectification of posture and trunk flexibility in children and adolescents] Haltung und Bewegung 1998 2:5-13.  [Google Scholar]

[12]Feldman AG, Ilmane N, Sangani S, Raptis H, Motor control and position sense: action-perception coupling Adv Exp Med Biol 2014 826:17-31.  [Google Scholar]

[13]Sousa AS, Silva A, Tavares JM, Biomechanical and neurophysiological mechanisms related to postural control and efficiency of movement: a review Somatosens Mot Res 2012 29(4):131-43.  [Google Scholar]

[14]Federolf P, Roos L, Nigg BM, Analysis of the multi-segmental postural movement strategies utilized in bipedal, tandem and one-leg stance as quantified by a principal component decomposition of marker coordinates Journal of biomechanics 2013 46(15):2626-33.  [Google Scholar]

[15]Kilby MC, Molenaar PC, Newell KM, Models of postural control: shared variance in joint and COM motions PloS one 2015 10(5):e0126379  [Google Scholar]

[16]Assaiante C, Mallau S, Viel S, Jover M, Schmitz C, Development of postural control in healthy children: a functional approach Neural Plast 2005 12(2-3):109-18.  [Google Scholar]

[17]Urquhart DM, Hodges PW, Allen TJ, Story IH, Abdominal muscle recruitment during a range of voluntary exercises Man Ther 2005 10(2):144-53.  [Google Scholar]

[18]Gade VK, Wilson SE, Position sense in the lumbar spine with torso flexion and loading Journal of applied biomechanics 2007 23(2):93-102.  [Google Scholar]

[19]Peterka RJ, Loughlin PJ, Dynamic regulation of sensorimotor integration in human postural control Journal of neurophysiology 2004 91(1):410-23.  [Google Scholar]

[20]Ting LH, Dimensional reduction in sensorimotor systems: a framework for understanding muscle coordination of posture Progress in brain research 2007 165:299-321.  [Google Scholar]

[21]Machado S, Cunha M, Velasques B, Minc D, Teixeira S, Domingues CA, Sensorimotor integration: basic concepts, abnormalities related to movement disorders and sensorimotor training-induced cortical reorganization Rev Neurol 2010 51(7):427-36.  [Google Scholar]

[22]Collins JJ, De Luca CJ, The effects of visual input on open-loop and closed-loop postural control mechanisms Exp Brain Res 1995 103(1):151-63.  [Google Scholar]

[23]Mallau S, Vaugoyeau M, Assaiante C, Postural strategies and sensory integration: no turning point between childhood and adolescence PloS one 2010 5(9):e13078  [Google Scholar]