JCDR - Register at Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research
Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research, ISSN - 0973 - 709X
Psychiatry/Mental Health Section DOI : 10.7860/JCDR/2019/37464.12456
Year : 2019 | Month : Jan | Volume : 13 | Issue : 01 Full Version Page : VC05 - VC09

Factor Structure of Schizotypal Personality in India

Sanjay Kumar1, Reshu Chaudhary2

1 Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, D.A.V. College, Muzaffarnagar, Uttar Pradesh, India.
2 Associate Professor, Department of Applied Sciences, Babu Banarsi Das Institute of Technology, Ghaziabad, Uttar Pradesh, India.


NAME, ADDRESS, E-MAIL ID OF THE CORRESPONDING AUTHOR: Dr. Sanjay Kumar, Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, D.A.V. College, Muzaffarnagar-251001, Uttar Pradesh, India.
E-mail: sanjaykumar.psy@gmail.com
Abstract

Introduction

Schizotypal personality represents genetic underpinning of schizophrenia spectrum disorders; hence, it provides conceptual models for understanding psychosis as well as a scheme for high-risk group identification. The study of structure of schizotypal personality is largely confined to western societies, whereas its assessment in varied socio-cultural groups is highly required.

Aim

To study the factor structure of schizotypal personality in an Indian population.

Materials and Methods

A sample of 492 college students (age, Mean= 21.3, SD= 2.61) filled the Hindi translated version of 74-items Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ). Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) technique was used to test the fitness (consonance of the modeled relationships, among latent and observed variables, in the sampled population) of different schizotypal personality models, that is, two-, three-, four-, bi-, and uni-factor models. The best-fit model was also studied for Measurement Invariance (MI) across gender groups.

Results

Three, four, and bi-factor models adequately fitted the data. Whereas, four-factor model was the best good-fit model. It also showed partially strong MI across gender groups. The internal consistency of total SPQ was 0.90 and of subscales ranged from 0.62 to 0.78. Men scored higher on several schizotypal facets but lower on social anxiety as compared to women.

Conclusion

The factor structure of schizotypal personality in India is similar to that reported elsewhere in the world. Thus, the present study supports the generalisation of schizotypal personality construct to the Indian people.

Keywords

Introduction

Schizotypal personality is a construct that is similar to traits of schizophrenia except for the extremely deleterious psychotic symptoms [1,2]. Consistent with the recent conceptualisation schemes [3-5], it may be conceived as a category representing personality disorder (schizotypy) as well as a set of traits present in the general population. Because a large number of studies report higher schizotypal personality scores in blood relatives and patients of schizophrenia [6-10]; genetic linkages [11,12]; and heritability in twins [13-14], schizotypal personality may represent genetic underpinning of schizophrenia spectrum disorders [15,16] or of psychosis in general [17,18]. The occurrence of structural and behavioural correlates, akin to schizophrenia, further supports this likelihood [19,20]. Thus, the study of schizotypal personality offers opportunity to understand the basis of schizophrenia spectrum disorders, free from confounds like medication and psychotic incapacitation, through robust large sample studies among healthy people [21]. It also provides scheme for identification of high-risk groups [3] as well as supports the identification of factors that defend against the culmination to psychosis [15].

The schizotypal personality is conceptualised as representing nine basic traits; that is, ideas of reference, magical thinking, unusual perceptual experiences, paranoid ideation, social anxiety, no close friends, blunted effect, odd behaviour, and odd speech [22]. It fails to organise as a uni-factor structure, although the exact number or the form of multifactor structure varies across populations, methods of study, and scales [23]. Some studies have reported two factors, namely, positive and negative schizotypy [24,25], whereas, the cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganised behaviour domains conception is the widely accepted one [26-31]. Recently, a comparatively better fit is reported for cognitive-perceptual, paranoid, interpersonal, and disorganised behaviour domains [32-37]. Moreover, a bi-factor structure with a common factor (for all the nine basic traits) and specific factors (negative and positive) has also been identified [21,38].

The 74-items forced-response choice (yes/no) based SPQ questionnaire, covering all the nine aspects, is a widely used tool for schizotypal personality assessment [39], translated and adapted for several cultures [27,29,37]. Both exploratory factor analytic and CFA studies report that Raine A et al., [30] three-factor model is a good fit for SPQ [10,17,27-29,40] showing invariance across sex [26,28,29,41,42] as well as across age [26,28,29]. This, along with the possibilities of invariance across different cultures and societies, supports identification of a cross-culturally valid schizotypal personality profile, which may be used in the development of criteria for the high-risk groups [3,4]. However, the assessment of schizotypal personality is limited in range and has inconsistencies because studies have reported a partial [21,29,36,43] or no invariance (in a shorter form of SPQ [44]) across cultures; most studies are limited to western countries [21], and environmental [45,46] and ethnic [47] factors do play a role in the occurrence of schizophrenia but their contributory effects are not clear. Hence, the assessment of schizotypal personality structure in more varied socio-cultural and regional groups is quite important [45].

No prior study attempted to study the structure of schizotypal personality in India. Earlier, Reynolds CA et al., did study an Indian population but that was in Mauritius and using Creole translated version of SPQ [29]. Thus, the present study attempted CFAs of the different structural models, proposed to be fitting to the schizotypal personality data, in a mainly "Hindi" speaking northern Indian population. On the basis of findings of earlier studies [35-36, 38], authors hypothesised that Raine A et al., [30] three-factor, Wuthrich VM and Bates TC’s [31] three-factor (modified model tested by Compton et al. [35]; W-Bm), Stefanis NC et al.’s [37] four-factor, and Preti A et al.’s [38] bi-factor models show adequate fitness with the schizotypal personality data, whereas Stefanis NC et al.’s [37] model is the best good-fit one [37,38]. Moreover, because invariance in the structure of multiple groups is an important construct validity test [48], authors further studied MI in gender groups for the best fitting model. The study of gender differences in schizotypal facets and internal consistency measurements of SPQ were the additional aims of the present study.

Materials and Methods

Participants: A total of 492 college students (age, Mean=21.3, SD=2.61), selected through opportunity sample, participated in this study conducted during the academic session of 2010-2011 (August to February) in a government-aided college of Muzaffarnagar city, Uttar Pradesh, India. There were 314 women (63%). Women (Mean=21.03, SD=2.67) were slightly younger than men (Mean=21.78, SD=2.45). Although no explicit information on socio-economic status was collected, most participants were expected to be from lower and lower-middle income sections of society because of the demography of this low-fees institution. Moreover, because a sample of >5-10 times of parameter variables is adequate [49], the sample size, in the present study, was appropriate for the second order CFA of SPQ.

Instrument: In the present study, a Hindi translated version of 74-items SPQ [39; supplementary material] was used to assess the schizotypal personality. For translation, a committee approach was followed. An initial draft, prepared by two psychologists, was presented to two English language teachers, for independent review, and to 10 students, for feedback on clarity and meaningfulness of the questionnaire. Based on their inputs, the committee of psychologists and language teachers approved the final draft of questionnaire. It was also sent to the original author (Raine A) for record [39]. The scoring procedure of SPQ involved awarding one score to each ‘yes’ response in the forced-response choice (yes/no) and calculating nine sub-factors of schizotypal personality by totalling relevant items.

Procedure: Although no Institutional Ethics Committee existed at the time of study, data collection procedure was largely guided by the principles laid down in Helsinki declaration for human subjects [50]. Participants gave an informed consent prior to the questionnaire administration. Moreover, participation was voluntary and without credits. A research assistant administered the questionnaire, in small groups (10-40), to students in their classroom and monitored them during the filling of questionnaire. Participants took 15 to 20 minutes on this task.

Statistical Analysis

For CFAs, authors used Satorra-Bentler’s (S-B’s) robust maximum likelihood method in LISREL 8.8, which corrects for the non-normality effects in data. Authors tested six CFA models: Seiver LJ and Gunderson JG’s [25] two-factor model, Raine A et al., [30] three-factor model, Stefanis NC et al.’s [37] four-factor model, Preti A et al.’s [38] bi-factor model, W-Bm [35] three-factor model, as well as, a basic, uni-factor model. The structure of relationships among variables, as proposed by the different models, may be apparent from [Table/Fig-1,2].

Standardised factor loadings and covariances for: a) Raine A, three-factor; b) Stefanis NC et al., four-factor; and c) W-B’s three-factor (modified) models.

Factors: CgP: Cognitive-perceptual, intP: Interpersonal, Ds: Disorganised, Pn: Paranoid and Neg: negative, Subscales: IOR: Idea-of-reference, UPE: Unusual perceptual experiences, OBMT: Odd beliefs/magical thinking, PI: Suspiciousness, ESA: Excessive social anxiety, NCF: No-close-friends, CA: Constricted affect, OS: Odd speech and OEB: Odd/eccentric behavior

Standardised factor loadings and covariances for Preti A et al., bi-factor; b) Siever LJ and Gunderson JG, two-factor; and c) uni-factor models.

Factors: Szt: Schizotypal, Pos: Positive, and Neg: Negative, Subscales: IOR: Idea-of-reference, UPE: Unusual perceptual experiences, OBMT: Odd beliefs/magical thinking, PI: Suspiciousness, ESA: Excessive social anxiety, NCF: No-close-friends, CA: Constricted affect, OS: Odd speech and OEB: Odd/eccentric behavior

The good-fitness of the models was decided on the basis of several a priori set indices. First, non-significant chi-square value shows the good-fitness of model; however, large sample size renders it ineffective (increases chances of significant difference). Second, the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), along with 90% confidence interval, is a robust criterion, which reports a reasonably fit model at smaller values (<0.08; good fit:< 0.06) when the 90% class interval range is below 0.1 [51]. Third, Standardised Root Mean Square (SRMR) reports goodness-of-fit at lower values (reasonably fit: < 0.08; good fit: < 0.06; [51]). Moreover, CFI and TLI are other criteria that report reasonable fit at values higher than 0.90 (good fit: > 0.95; [51]). Lastly, the lower values of Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) scale the comparative fitness of different models, especially in non-hierarchical model testing [51].

For MI testing between multiple groups, models with increasing levels of constraints are hierarchically tested {48; flow chart in [Table/Fig-3]}. As a first step, the configural invariance is tested in which the same model is constrained to occur in different subgroups. If configural invariance is established, it means that same model is good-fit in different subgroups, and therefore, the pattern of relationship between the variables is similar in different subgroups. The configural invariance is a prerequisite for testing higher order similarities or equivalences. In the second step, the weak (metric) MI model is tested by constraining the factor loadings (the measure of relationship of observed variable with underlying ‘latent’ factor). If the modelling indexes of MI and lower (here configural) models are within range, the constrained parameters of the higher model are considered invariant. Earlier chi-square difference value was used as such index, but it has problem of sensitivity to the sample size. Hence, in the present study authors used the difference between CFI values of MI and lower models (^CFI < 0.01; ^ denotes difference) as criterion for equivalence testing [52]. After weak MI, the next step is to test the strong MI (scalar) model, in which intercepts are additionally constrained for equality. Thus, if the ^CFI value, between scalar and lower (metric) models, is lower than 0.01, strong MI is established. However, in case, higher MI model is worse than lower (metric or configural) model, partially invariant (weak or strong) models should be established by relaxing the equality constraints on some of the parameters. Moreover, although strict MI’s (constraining factor variances, factor covariances, and residual error variances) have been proposed, they are considered unrealistic. Thus, practically, invariance testing terminates after strong MI [48].

Flow chart of the plan of hierarchical measurement invariance testing followed in the present study.

Studies have reported that the standard methods for analysis of nominal and ordinal data severely affect the MI testing in lower sample size studies [53,54], whereas, the Maximum Likelihood (ML) method is robust against non-normality in invariance testing in small samples [54,55] and it does not inflate chi square differences for mixed-item formats and sample size combinations [56]. Thus, authors used ML method for the MI testing across gender groups in the present study.

The internal consistency for non-continuous psychological scales (ordinal and nominal) is better assessed by omega reliability coefficient [57]. Therefore, in the present study, authors reported omega reliability of SPQ and its subscales, calculated using R software. Moreover, the mean differences in gender groups were calculated using SPSS version 17.0 program. Authors used MANCOVA for the study of gender differences in schizotypal facets after controlling for the possible confounding age effects.

Results

The goodness-of-fit indices for different models fitting the total sample are given in [Table/Fig-4]. In accordance with authors’ expectations, three-, four-, and bi-factor models were reasonably fit models on all goodness-of-fit indices (RMSEA, SRMR, CFI, & TLI). Whereas, two- and uni-factor models were below the acceptable level of fit indices (RMSEA). Furthermore, as per the AIC indices, the Stefanis NC et al., four-factor model was the best good-fit amongst all models [37]. This is supported by other indices also (RMSEA, CFI, and TLI; exception, SRMR). [Table/Fig-1,2] show the values of factor loadings and factor correlates of these models.

Goodness-of-fit indices for six models studied.

S-Bχ2dfp-valueCFITLISRMRRMSEA (90% CI)AIC
One factor (F) model238.8227<0.0010.920.90.0720.13 (0.11-0.14)274.82
Siever LJ and Gunderson JG, 2F model167.9026<0.0010.950.930.0620.11 (0.091-0.12)205.90
Raine A, 3F model89.6623<0.0010.980.960.0490.077 (0.060-0.094)133.66
Stefanis NC et al., 4F model40.44190.0030.990.980.0360.048 (0.027-0.068)92.44
Preti A et al., Bi-F model76.7718<0.0010.980.960.0410.082 (0.063-0.10)130.77
WBm 3F model71.6921<0.0010.980.970.0340.07 (0.053-0.088)119.69

SBχ2: Satorra-bentler corrected chi-square; df: Degrees of freedom; CFI: Comparative fit index; TLI: Tucker-lewis index; SRMR: Standardised root mean square residual; RMSEA (90% CI): Steiger-lind root mean square error of approximation (90% class interval); and AIC: Akaike information criteria


Because invariance of the factor structure in subgroups of a population is an important test for the criterion validity, authors studied MI across gender groups for the best-fitting four-factor model [Table/Fig-5]. Hierarchical testing, reported configural and metric invariance but not scalar invariance. Thus, attempts were made to identify a partially strong MI model. Freeing the intercepts of excessive-social-anxiety and odd-eccentric-behaviour from the equality constraints led to the identification of a partially strong MI model (^CFI < .01). Hence, gender groups had similar factor structure and factor loadings, whereas intercepts were equivalent only after discounting excessive social anxiety and odd-eccentric behaviour.

Fit indices of configural, weak, strong, and partially-strong MI models for Stefanis NC et al.’s four factor model.

χ2dfCFITLISRMRRMSEA (90% CI)AIC2^df^CFI
Configural74.37380.9850.9720.02780.0625 (0.041; 0.083)178.365
Weak MI87.16450.9820.9710.03460.0618 (0.042; 0.081)213.16412.7970.003
Strong MI157.62540.9580.9440.03450.0885 (0.073; 0.105)265.61770.4690.024
Partial Strong MI109.44520.9750.9650.0340.0671(0.05; 0.085)221.43822.2870.007
(OEB & ESA intercepts freed)

χ2: Chi-square; df: Degrees of freedom; CFI: Comparative fit index; TLI: Tucker-lewis index; SRMR: Standardised root mean square residual; RMSEA (90% CI): Steiger-lind root mean square error of approximation (90% class interval); AIC: Akaike information criteria; ^χ2: Difference of chi-square values; ^Df: Difference of degree of freedom values; ^CFI: Difference of comparative fit index values; ESA: Excessive social anxiety; and OEB: Odd/eccentric behavior


The internal consistency estimates for the whole scale was 0.90 and for the subscales 0.62 to 0.78 [Table/Fig-6]. After controlling for the effect of age, MANCOVA analysis showed a significant effect of gender, F(9, 481)=7.13, p<.001. Subsequent univariate analyses showed that the men had higher odd beliefs, unusual perception, odd behaviour, odd speech, constricted affect, and suspiciousness, but lower social anxiety, than women [Table/Fig-6].

Descriptive statistics, uni-variate analyses of variance, and effect sizes of differences between men and women, as well as internal consistency coefficients for SPQ sub-scales.

MenWomenANOVA (1, 489)*Effect Size!Consistency
FacetsMSDMSDFp-valuedΩ (Total)
IOR4.842.034.672.270.90.30.080.68
ESA2.842.043.312.273.90.049-0.210.78
OBMT2.981.592.641.724.40.040.200.66
UPE3.451.752.751.8817.3<0.0010.380.62
OEB3.001.862.011.9133.1<0.0010.520.78
NCF3.361.943.012.183.70.0550.170.65
OS3.282.172.722.3180.0050.250.75
CA2.871.722.401.818.50.0040.260.65
PI4.421.724.092.214.40.040.160.72
SPT31.039.827.612.72--0.290.90

IOR: Idea-of-reference; ESA: Excessive social anxiety; OBMT: Odd beliefs/magical thinking; UPE: Unusual perceptual experiences; OEB: Odd/eccentric behaviour; NCF: No-close-friends; OS: Odd speech; CA: Constricted affect; PI: Suspiciousness; and SPT: Schizotypal personality total score; *after controlling the effect of age; !Positive effect size value indicates that men have higher score than women, whereas negative value indicates the opposite pattern


Discussion

Similar to prior studies on healthy populations [27-38], the present study reports adequate-to-good fitness of three-, four-, and bi-factor models. Moreover, it identifies four-factor model as the best good-fit model, similar to earlier studies [34-37]. Thus, the present study strongly supports the likelihood that the structure of schizotypal personality traits in India is similar to that reported elsewhere in the world. This, in turn, also supports the general acceptability of SPQ.

Furthermore, the present study strengthens the recently growing support for the conception of schizotypal personality in terms of cognitive-perceptual, paranoid, interpersonal, and disorganised behaviour domains [32-37,58]. However, because clinical [6-7,13] and exploratory factor analytic [10,27-30,40] studies support the cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganised behaviour domains conception and because four-factor model has psychometric difficulties (loading of multiple facets on to the multiple factors [21]), the need of search for better structural model of schizotypal personality cannot be denied. Perhaps, the exploratory structural equation modeling and translational studies holds prospects of developing better conceptualization of schizotypal personality [3].

Also, authors found partially strong MI for four-factor model across gender groups. Thus, men and women have similar meaning of schizotypal personality and have similar strength of its structural relationships. In addition, with the exception of excessive social anxiety and odd-eccentric behaviour, the differences in observed mean scores express the differences in mean values of latent constructs similarly in men and women. Earlier studies also have reported partial strong [58] and strong [41] MI in gender groups for Stefanis NC et al., four factor model [37]. Thus, the present study lends additional support to the construct validity of SPQ.

The present study also reports gender differences in the sub-scales of SPQ, that is, men having higher unusual perception, odd speech, odd behaviour, constricted affect, and lower social anxiety than women. This is consistent with the earlier reports [28,31,34]. Thus, the pattern of gender differences in schizotypal personality in India is largely similar to that reported in other countries. Furthermore, because the meaning of schizotypal personality is the same across gender groups, these differences may be reflecting the real effects of developmental or cultural factors.

Limitation

The present study has following limitations. Firstly, instead of the first-order factor analysis (based on SPQ items), it involves a second-order factor analysis (based on derived sub-factors). However, because the nine sub-factors classification of schizotypal personality is valid and the first-order factor analysis requires a large sample size; the present second-order factor analysis is justified.

Lastly, no attempt has been made to assess the psychiatric problems among the young participants, which represents a highly vulnerable age group. However, because the college-going population is expected to have comparatively lesser psychiatric problems and because it has been extensively focused by earlier studies, authors can expect a lesser atypical sample in the present study. Thus, whereas SPQ may serve as a tool for early identification of the schizophrenia susceptibility of the students’ population, community-based studies, corroborated by diagnostic interviews, are required for its widespread use in India.

Conclusion

The factor structure of schizotypal personality in India is similar to that reported elsewhere in the world. The meaning of schizotypal personality (as measured by SPQ) is similarly structured by men and women. Moreover, the pattern of gender differences in schizotypal personality is also similar to that reported in other parts of the world. Thus, the present study supports the generalisation of the schizotypal personality construct to the Indian people.

Supplementary Material

The supplementary material is available at: https://jcdr.net/articles/supplementarydata/12456/37464_SPQ-hindi.pdf.

SBχ2: Satorra-bentler corrected chi-square; df: Degrees of freedom; CFI: Comparative fit index; TLI: Tucker-lewis index; SRMR: Standardised root mean square residual; RMSEA (90% CI): Steiger-lind root mean square error of approximation (90% class interval); and AIC: Akaike information criteriaχ2: Chi-square; df: Degrees of freedom; CFI: Comparative fit index; TLI: Tucker-lewis index; SRMR: Standardised root mean square residual; RMSEA (90% CI): Steiger-lind root mean square error of approximation (90% class interval); AIC: Akaike information criteria; ^χ2: Difference of chi-square values; ^Df: Difference of degree of freedom values; ^CFI: Difference of comparative fit index values; ESA: Excessive social anxiety; and OEB: Odd/eccentric behaviorIOR: Idea-of-reference; ESA: Excessive social anxiety; OBMT: Odd beliefs/magical thinking; UPE: Unusual perceptual experiences; OEB: Odd/eccentric behaviour; NCF: No-close-friends; OS: Odd speech; CA: Constricted affect; PI: Suspiciousness; and SPT: Schizotypal personality total score; *after controlling the effect of age; !Positive effect size value indicates that men have higher score than women, whereas negative value indicates the opposite pattern

References

[1]Gruzelier JH, The factorial structure of schizotypy: Part I. Affinities with syndromes of schizophrenia Schizophr Bull 1996 22:611-20.10.1093/schbul/22.4.6118938915  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[2]Nelson MT, Seal ML, Pantelis C, Phillips LJ, Evidence of a dimensional relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia: a systematic review Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013 37:317-27.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.00423313650  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[3]Fonseca Pedrero E, Debbané M, Schizotypal traits and psychotic-like experiences during adolescence: An update Psicothema 2017 29:5-17.  [Google Scholar]

[4]Kwapil TR, Barrantes-Vidal N, Schizotypy: looking back and moving forward Schizophr Bull 2015 41:S366-73.10.1093/schbul/sbu18625548387  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[5]van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L, A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder Psychol Med 2009 39:179-95.10.1017/S003329170800381418606047  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[6]Kendler KS, Gruenberg AM, Strauss JS, An independent analysis of the Copenhagen sample of the Danish adoption study of schizophrenia. II. The relationship between schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981 38:982-84.10.1001/archpsyc.1981.017803400340037283669  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[7]Kendler KS, McGuire M, Gruenberg AM, O’Hare A, Spellman M, Walsh D, The Roscommon Family Study. I. Methods, diagnosis of probands, and risk of schizophrenia in relatives Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993 50:527-40.10.1001/archpsyc.1993.018201900290048317947  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[8]Tarbox SI, Pogue-Geile MF, A multivariate perspective on schizotypy and familial association with schizophrenia: a review Clin Psychol Rev 2011 31:1169-82.10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.00221855827  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[9]Torgersen S, Edvardsen J, Øien PA, Onstad S, Skre I, Lygren S, Schizotypal personality disorder inside and outside the schizophrenic spectrum Schizophr Res 2002 54:33-38.10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00349-8  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[10]Vollema MG, Sitskoorn MM, Appels MC, Kahn RS, Does the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire reflect the biological-genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia? Schizophr Res 2002 54:39-45.10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00350-4  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[11]Stefanis NC, Trikalinos TA, Avramopoulos D, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Ntzani EE, Impact of schizophrenia candidate genes on schizotypy and cognitive endophenotypes at the population level Biol Psychiatry 2007 62:784-92.10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.01517336946  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[12]Yasuda Y, Hashimoto R, Ohi K, Fukumoto M, Umeda-Yano S, Yamamori H, Impact on schizotypal personality trait of a genome-wide supported psychosis variant of the ZNF804A gene Neurosci Lett 2011 495:216-20.21457757  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[13]Battaglia M, Fossati A, Torgersen S, Bertella S, Bajo S, Maffei C, A psychometric-genetic study of schizotypal disorder Schizophr Res 1999 37:53-64.10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00131-5  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[14]Linney YM, Murray RM, Peters ER, MacDonald AM, Rijsdijk F, Sham PC, A quantitative genetic analysis of schizotypal personality traits Psychol Med 2003 33:803-16.10.1017/S0033291703007906   [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[15]Barrantes-Vidal N, Grant P, Kwapil TR, The role of schizotypy in the study of the etiology of schizophrenia spectrum disorders Schizophr Bull 2015 41:S408-16.10.1093/schbul/sbu19125810055  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[16]Fanous AH, Neale MC, Gardner CO, Webb BT, Straub RE, O’Neill FA, Significant correlation in linkage signals from genome-wide scans of schizophrenia and schizotypy Mol Psychiatry 2007 12:958-65.10.1038/sj.mp.400199617440434  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[17]Bergman AJ, Harvey PD, Mitropoulou V, Aronson A, Marder D, Silverman J, The factor structure of schizotypal symptoms in a clinical population Schizophr Bull 1996 22:501-09.10.1093/schbul/22.3.5018873300  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[18]Lewandowski KE, Barrantes-Vidal N, Nelson-Gray RO, Clancy C, Kepley HO, Kwapil TR, Anxiety and depression symptoms in psychometrically identified schizotypy Schizophr Res 2006 83:225-35.10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.02416448805  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[19]DeRosse P, Nitzburg GC, Ikuta T, Peters BD, Malhotra AK, Szeszko PR, Evidence from structural and diffusion tensor imaging for frontotemporal deficits in psychometric schizotypy Schizophr Bull 2015 41:104-14.10.1093/schbul/sbu15025392520  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[20]Siddi S, Petretto DR, Preti A, Neuropsychological correlates of schizotypy: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2017 22:186-212.10.1080/13546805.2017.129970228288547  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[21]Fonseca-Pedrero E, Debbané M, Ortuño-Sierra J, Chan RCK, Cicero DC, Zhang LC, The structure of schizotypal personality traits: a cross-national study Psychol Med 2018 48:451-62.10.1017/S003329171700182928712364  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[22]American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 2013 Fifth EditionArlington VAAmerican Psychiatric Association10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[23]Fonseca-Pedrero E, Ortuño-Sierra J, Sierro G, Daniel C, Cella M, Preti A, The measurement invariance of schizotypy in Europe Eur Psychiatry 2015 30:837-44.10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.07.00526443051  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[24]Kendler KS, Ochs AL, Gorman AM, Hewitt JK, Ross DE, Mirsky AF, The structure of schizotypy: a pilot multitrait twin study Psychiatry Res 1991 36:19-36.10.1016/0165-1781(91)90114-5  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[25]Siever LJ, Gunderson JG, The search for a schizotypal personality: historical origins and current status Compr Psychiatry 1983 24:199-212.10.1016/0010-440X(83)90070-6  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[26]Badcock J, Dragovic M, Schizotypal personality in mature adults Personality and Individual Differences 2006 40:77-85.10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.015  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[27]Chen WJ, Hsiao CK, Lin CC, Schizotypy in community samples: the three-factor structure and correlation with sustained attention J Abnorm Psychol 1997 106:649-54.10.1037/0021-843X.106.4.649  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[28]Fossati A, Raine A, Carretta I, Leonardi B, Maffei C, The three-factor model of schizotypal personality: Invariance across age and gender Personality and Individual Differences 2003 35:1007-19.10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00314-8  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[29]Reynolds CA, Raine A, Mellingen K, Venables PH, Mednick SA, Three-factor model of schizotypal personality: Invariance across culture, gender, religious affiliation, family adversity, and psychopathology Schizophr Bull 2000 26:603-18.10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a03348110993401  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[30]Raine A, Reynolds C, Lencz T, Scerbo A, Triphon N, Kim D, Cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized features of schizotypal personality Schizophr Bull 1994 20:191-201.10.1093/schbul/20.1.1918197415  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[31]Wuthrich VM, Bates TC, Confirmatory factor analysis of the three-factor structure of the schizotypal personality questionnaire and Chapman schizotypy scales J Pers Assess 2006 87:292-304.10.1207/s15327752jpa8703_1017134337  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[32]Barron D, Swami V, Towell T, Hutchinson G, Morgan KD, Examination of the factor structure of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire among British and Trinidadian adults Biomed Res Int 2015 2015:25827510.1155/2015/25827525699263  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[33]Barron D, Morgan KD, Towell T, Jaafar JL, Swami V, Psychometric properties of the Malay Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire: Measurement invariance and latent mean comparisons in Malaysian adults Asia Pac Psychiatry 2018 :1010.1111/appy.1229328677341  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[34]Bora E, BaysanArabaci L, Confirmatory factor analysis of schizotypal personality traits in university students Turkish Journal of Psychiatry 2009 20:339-45.  [Google Scholar]

[35]Compton MT, Goulding SM, Bakeman R, McClure-Tone EB, Confirmation of a four-factor structure of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire among undergraduate students Schizophr Res 2009 111:46-52.10.1016/j.schres.2009.02.01219278834  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[36]Fonseca-Pedrero E, Compton MT, Tone EB, Ortuño-Sierra J, Paino M, Fumero A, Cross-cultural invariance of the factor structure of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire across Spanish and American college students Psychiatry Res 2014 220:1071-76.10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.05025412981  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[37]Stefanis NC, Smyrnis N, Avramopoulos D, Evdokimidis I, Ntzoufras I, Stefanis CN, Factorial composition of self-rated schizotypal traits among young males undergoing military training Schizophr Bull 2004 30:335-50.10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a00708315279051  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[38]Preti A, Siddi S, Vellante M, Scanu R, Muratore T, Gabrielli M, Bifactor structure of the schizotypal personality questionnaire (SPQ) Psychiatry Res 2015 230:940-50.10.1016/j.psychres.2015.11.01026607431  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[39]Raine A, The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria Schizophr Bull 1991 17:555-64.10.1093/schbul/17.4.5551805349  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[40]Claridge G, McCreery C, Mason O, Bentall R, Boyle G, Slade P, The factor structure of “schizotypal’ traits: a large replication study Br J Clin Psychol 1996 35:103-15.10.1111/j.2044-8260.1996.tb01166.x  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[41]Tsaousis I, Zouraraki C, Karamaouna P, Karagiannopoulou L, Giakoumaki SG, The validity of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire in a Greek sample: Tests of measurement invariance and latent mean differences Compr Psychiatry 2015 62:51-62.10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.06.00326343467  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[42]Yu J, Bernardo AB, Zaroff CM, Chinese version of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire: Factor structure replication and invariance across sex Asia Pac Psychiatry 2016 8:226-37.10.1111/appy.1221526440145  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[43]Cicero DC, Measurement invariance of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire in Asian, Pacific Islander, White, and multiethnic populations Psychol Assess 2016 28:351-61.10.1037/pas000018026121382  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[44]Liu S, Mellor D, Ling M, Saiz JL, Vinet EV, Xu X, The Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief lacks measurement invariance across three countries Psychiatry Res 2017 258:544-50.10.1016/j.psychres.2017.08.08828899612  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[45]Cohen AS, Mohr C, Ettinger U, Chan RC, Park S, Schizotypy as an organizing framework for social and affective sciences Schizophr Bull 2015 41:S427-35.10.1093/schbul/sbu19525810057  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[46]Van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP, The environment and schizophrenia Nature 2010 468:203-12.10.1038/nature0956321068828  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[47]Chavira DA, Grilo CM, Shea MT, Yen S, Gunderson JG, Morey LC, Ethnicity and four personality disorders Compr Psychiatry 2003 44:483-91.10.1016/S0010-440X(03)00104-4  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[48]Byrne BM, Structural Equation Modeling With AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming 2010 Second Edition (Multivariate Applications Series)New YorkRoutledge, Taylor & Francis group  [Google Scholar]

[49]Bentler PM, Chu CP, Practical issues in structural modeling Sociol Methods Res 1987 16:78-117.10.1177/0049124187016001004  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[50]World Medical AssociationWorld Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects J Postgrad Med 2002 48(3):206-08.  [Google Scholar]

[51]Hu L, Bentler P, Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives Struct Equ Modeling 1999 6:1-55.10.1080/10705519909540118  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[52]Cheung GW, Rensvold RB, Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance Struct Equ Modeling 2002 9:233-55.10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[53]French BF, Finch HW, Confirmatory factor analytic procedures for the determination of measurement invariance Struct Equ Modeling 2006 13:378-402.10.1207/s15328007sem1303_3  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[54]Finch HW, French BF, Hernández Finch ME, Comparison of methods for factor invariance testing of a 1-factor model with small samples and skewed latent traits Front Psychol 2018 9:33210.3389/fpsyg.2018.0033229623053  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[55]Sass DA, Schmitt TA, Marsh HW, Evaluating model fit with ordered categorical data within a measurement invariance framework: A comparison of estimators Struct Equ Modeling 2014 21:167-80.10.1080/10705511.2014.882658   [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[56]Koh KH, Zumbo BD, Multi-group confirmatory factor analysis for testing measurement invariance in mixed item format data J Mod Appl Stat Methods 2008 7:471-77.10.22237/jmasm/1225512660  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]

[57]Trizano-Hermosilla I, Alvarado JM, Best alternatives to Cronbach’s alpha reliability in realistic conditions: Congeneric and asymmetrical measurements Front Psychol 2016 7:76910.3389/fpsyg.2016.0076927303333  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]

[58]Barron D, Voracek M, Tran US, Ong HS, Morgan KD, Towell T, A reassessment of the higher-order factor structure of the German Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ-G) in German-speaking adults Psychiatry Res 2018 269:328-36.10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.07030173038  [Google Scholar]  [CrossRef]  [PubMed]